Page:1902 Encyclopædia Britannica - Volume 27 - CHI-ELD.pdf/177

This page needs to be proofread.

bionomics.]

COLOURS

OF

examples of Mimicry among tropical American butterflies were discussed by Bates in bis paper; and in 1866 A. 11. Wallace extended the hypothesis to the butterflies of the tropical East {Trans. Linn. Soc. vol. xxv., 1866, p. 19); Roland Trimen {Trans. Linn. Soc. vol. xxvi., 1870, p. 497) to those of Africa in 1870. The term Mimicry is used in various senses. It is often extended, as indeed it was by Bates, to include all the superficial resemblances between animals and any part of their environment. Wallace, however, separated the Cryptic Resemblances already described, and the majority of naturalists have followed this convenient arrangement. In Cryptic Resemblance an animal resembles some object of no interest to its enemy (or prey), and in so doing is concealed; in Mimicry an animal resembles some other animal which is specially disliked by its enemy, or some object which is specially attractive to its prey, and in so doing becomes conspicuous. Some naturalists have considered Mimicry to include all superficial likenesses between animals, but such a classification would group together resemblances which have widely different uses. (1) The resemblance of a mollusc to the coral on which it lives, or an external parasite to the hair or skin of its host, would be Procryptic ; (2) that between moths which resemble lichen, Syncryptic; (3) between distasteful insects, Synaposematic; (4) between the Insectivor mole and the Rodent mole-rat, Syntechnic; (5) the essential element in Mimicry is that it is a false warning (Pseud-aposematic) or false recognition (Pseudepisematic) character. Some have considered that Mimicry indicates resemblance to a moving object; but apart from the non-mimetic likenesses between animals classified above, there are ordinary Cryptic Resemblances to drifting leaves, swaying bits of twig, &c., while truly Mimetic Resemblances are often specially adapted for the attitude of rest. Many use the term Mimicry to include Synaposematic as well as Pseudo-sematic Resemblances, calling the former “Mullerian,” the latter “Batesian,” Mimicry. The objection to this grouping is that it takes little account of the deceptive element which is essential in Mimicry. In Synaposematic colouring the warning is genuine, in Pseudaposematic it is a sham. The term Mimicry has led to much misunderstanding from the fact that in ordinary speech it implies deliberate imitation. The production of Mimicry in an individual animal has no more to do with consciousness or “ taking thought ” than any of the other processes of growth. Protective Mimicry is here defined as an advantageous and superficial resemblance of one animal to another, which latter is specially defended so as to be disliked or feared by the majority of enemies of the groups to which both belong—a resemblance which appeals to the sense of sight, sometimes to that of hearing, and rarely to smell, but does not extend to deep-seated characters except when the superficial likeness is affected by them. Mutatis mutandis this definition will apply to Aggressive (Pseudepisematic) Resemblance. The conditions under which Mimicry occurs have been stated by Wallace :—“(1) that the imitative species occur in the same area and occupy the same station as the imitated; (2) that the imitators are always the more defenceless; (3) that the imitators are always less numerous in individuals ; (4) that the imitators differ from the bulk of their allies ; (5) that the imitation, however minute, is external and visible only, never extending to internal characters or to such as do not affect the external appearance.” It is obvious that conditions 2 and 3 do not hold in the case of Mullerian Mimicry. Mimicry has been explained, independently of Natural Selection, by the supposition that it is the common expression of the direct action of common causes, such as climate, food, &c.; also by the supposition of independent lines of evolution leading to the same result without any selective action in

ANIMALS

149

consequence of advantage in the struggle; also by the operation of Sexual Selection. It is proposed, in conclusion, to give an account of the broad aspects of Mimicry, and attempt a brief discussion of the theories of origin of each class of facts (see Poulton, Linn. Soc. Journ. Zool., 1898, p. 558). It will be found that in many cases the argument here made use of applies equally to the origin of Cryptic and Sematic Colours. The relationship between these classes has been explained: Mimicry is, as Wallace has stated {Darwinism, London, 1889), merely “an exceptional form of protective resemblance.” Now, protective (cryptic) resemblance cannot be explained on any of the lines suggested above, except natural selection; even sexual selection fails, because cryptic resemblance is especially common in the immature stages of insect life. But it would be unreasonable to explain Mimetic Resemblance by one set of principles and Cryptic by another and totally different set. Again, it may be plausible to explain the mimicry of one butterfly for another on one of the suggested lines, but the resemblance of a fly or moth to a wasp is by no means so easy, and here selection would be generally conceded ; yet the appeal to antagonistic principles to explain such closely related cases would only be justified by much direct evidence. Furthermore, the mimetic resemblances between butterflies are not haphazard, but the models almost invariably belong only to certain sub-families, the Danaince and Acraeinoe in all the warmer parts of the world, and, in tropical America, the Ithomiince and HeliconincB as well. These groups have the characteristics of Aposematic species, and no theory but natural selection explains their invariable occurrence as models wherever they exist. It is impossible to suggest, except by natural selection, any explanation of the fact that mimetic resemblances are confined to changes which produce or strengthen a superficial likeness. Very deep-seated changes are generally involved, inasmuch as the appropriate instincts as. to attitude, &c., are as important as colour and marking. The same conclusion is reached when we analyse the nature of mimetic resemblance and realize how complex it really is, being made up of colours, both pigmentary and structural, pattern, form, attitude, and movement. A plausible interpretation of colour may be wildly improbable when applied to some other element, and there is no explanation except natural selection which can explain all these elements. The appeal to the direct action of local conditions in common often breaks down upon the slightest investigation, the difference in habits between mimic and model in the same locality causing the most complete divergence in their conditions of life. Thus many insects produced from burrowing larvae mimic those whose larvae live in the open. Mimetic resemblance is far commoner in the female than in the male, a fact readily explicable by selection, as suggested by Wallace, for the female is compelled to fly more slowly and to expose itself while laying eggs, and hence a resemblance to the slow-flying freely exposed models is especially advantageous. The facts that mimetic species occur in the same locality, fly at the same time of the year as their models, and are day-flying species even though they may belong to nocturnal groups, are also more or less difficult to explain except on the theory of natural selection, and so also is the fact that mimetic resemblance is produced in the most varied manner. A spider resembles its model, an ant, by a modification of its body-form into a superficial resemblance, and by holding one pair of legs to repiesent antennae; certain bugs (Hemiptera) and beetles have also gained a shape unusual in their respective groups, a shape which superficially resembles an ant; a Locustid (Myrmecophana) has the shape of an ant. painted, as it were, on its body, all other parts resembling the back-